After all the spelling mistakes, after all the groping in the dark,
Can this page of strange gibberish get a final punctuation mark?
Hurry up and let's get this over with.
You don't have to go home but you can't stay here.
Everybody knows how this goes so let's get over it, and let's get this over with.

EL 2100 3100

PSYC 3100 sec 001
History And Systems Of Psychology, Spring 2019
UConn Storrs Campus, GENT 131
MON WED 4:40-5:55
Eric Lundquist


Hey remember, the first thing you'll see when I'm done with grades is your FINAL COURSE GRADE appearing on StudentAdmin on Monday or Tuesday. Until then I really won't have any opportunity to respond to individual emails wondering when the grades will be posted. Once the course grades are posted, I will add the rest of the scores to HuskyCT, hopefully within a week or so, and then you'll have all the information about where your course grade came from. If you still have questions after all the scores are added to HuskyCT, you can certainly ask then. Thanks for your patience!


FINAL EXAM REVIEW INFO
This is a preliminary version and may be updated after the review session Friday 5/3/19, 3:00 in BOUS A-106. Probably won't change much though.

FINAL EXAM IS MONDAY 5/6/19, 1:00-3:00 PM, GENT 131
REVIEW SESSION FRIDAY 5/3/19, 3:00-4:30, BOUS A-106

RECORDING OF EXAM 2 REVIEW SESSION FRIDAY 5/3/19 (approx. 77 minutes / 37 MB)
(starts off slow and rambling, ends up being kind of worthwhile once the questions get going -- but mainly as an extra review once you've done some studying)

Recording of brief lecture on epistemology to supplement readings on Descartes, Locke, Berkeley, Hume, and Kant


QUIZ 2 IS POSTED HERE (as a web page) and also HERE (as a PDF file for printing).

INSTEAD OF FILLING OUT A BUBBLE SHEET WITH YOUR 20 ANSWERS, RECORD THEM INTO THIS GOOGLE FORM LINK AND CLICK "SUBMIT" WHEN FINISHED!
* Note that the text of the questions does not appear on this form, only a link to the questions. So be careful to click on the answer you mean to give for each question -- just like on a bubble sheet.
* You must be logged into your UConn Google account, NOT your personal Google account. Ignore the "REQUEST EDIT ACCESS" button, that's only for people who collaborate with me on editing the form, which you are not. Doing the form from some phones may seem to require this; in that case, do the form from a computer instead.
* If you make a mistake, you cannot go back and edit your submission, but you CAN fill out the entire form again and I will only count the last set of answers you submit. But really, treat it like a bubble sheet that you don't get a second chance on once it's submitted -- record your answers accurately in the first place!

Due date for Quiz 2 Form submissions is Thursday 5/2/19. You may use the web page, the textbook, and your notes, but you may NOT work together on this quiz.

Please notice that I have partly re-written almost every question or combined it with someone else's submission. SO IF YOU THINK YOU RECOGNIZE YOUR OWN QUESTION, BE ESPECIALLY CAREFUL NOT TO ASSUME THE ANSWER IS THE SAME ONE YOU SUBMITTED. You can always compare it to the one you wrote to be sure.

I recommend that you do the quiz immediately (and quickly) without studying to see how you do with the material, then do it again more carefully using the resources listed above while learning that material. After all, the final exam will include these topics as well, so I'd hope completing this quiz would be a form of studying for the exam.

QUIZ 2 INFO
READ IMMEDIATELY!... how to submit a question; bubble sheets; planned online posting date; planned due date; etc.
Note that subject matter for the quiz extends into reading we haven't covered yet. This may be updated depending on the timing of the quiz posting and due date.


EXAM 1 REVIEW INFO
UPDATE: Popper and Kuhn will not be on the exam. Logical Positivism could be on it a little, since it was discussed in the context of Philosophical Behaviorism, so you should see the portions of the "Outline of Logical Positivism" indicated below (omitting the mention of Popper and Kuhn, as well as all its predecessors). The Stanovich reading on defining science, and the corresponding slides (up to "publicly observable data", but not Einstein etc.) are fair game since they're the last thing we covered.

EXAM 1 IS WEDNESDAY 4/3/19, 4:40-5:55 PM., GENT 131
REVIEW SESSION MONDAY 4/1/19, 6:00-7:00 PM (ending earlier or later depending on time needed), GENT 131 (not BOUS 160!).

RECORDING OF EXAM 1 REVIEW SESSION MONDAY 4/1/19 (approx. 62 minutes / 30 MB)


QUIZ 1 REVIEW INFO


UConn Microaggressions Survey: a campus-wide survey being run in part by members of the Psychology Department. The more responses the better, and it's looking at important questions related to experiences of racial discrimination so you might consider spending a half hour doing it. This study is completely unconnected to this course and is not worth any points or extra credit toward your grade, so you'd only be doing it to help science!

[phrenology picture] [sarcasm picture]



E-mail: Eric.Lundquist@uconn.edu
Office: BOUS 136
Office Hours: Mon Wed 6:00-7:00, and by appointment
Phone: (860) 486-4084


READING:

  1. REQUIRED: Hergenhahn, B.R. (2009). An Introduction to the History of Psychology (6th ed.). Wadsworth. (ISBN13: 978-0-495-50621-8) OR SEVENTH EDITION
  2. REQUIRED: On-Line Readings and Reserve Readings (to be announced)
  3. OPTIONAL: Some classic papers in the Psychology of Learning: Here's a collection of links to papers I'll refer to in class -- and a few of them may appear among the required readings for the class. Also see Classics In The History Of Psychology, if you're looking for extra stuff to read.

GRADING:
   
  • Two Quizzes:
  • 30%   approximately 5th and 12th weeks of class (Thursday 2/20/19 and Thursday 4/17/19)
    NOTE: QUIZ 2 is a TAKE-HOME quiz that will be posted sometime after 4/17/19 and will (most likely) be due on 4/24/19 though that could be later; details to appear on web page.
       
  • Midterm Exam:
  • 35%   approximately 10th week of class (Wed 4/3)
       
  • Final Exam:
  • 35%   MONDAY 5/6/19 1:00 PM IN GENT 131

    Explanation of grade calculation for this course
    Spreadsheet for calculating grades for this course
    Spreadsheet for calculating the effect of a specific course grade on overall GPA

    Other general University policies and information are available at the Office of the Provost's page of References for Syllabi Links, including policies concerning such issues as Absences from Final Examinations, Class Attendance, Credit Hours, People with Disabilities, Discrimination, Harassment and Related Interpersonal Violence, The Student Code, Academic Misconduct Procedures for Instructors, and more. These should be considered part of every course's policies at UConn.


    CLASS SYLLABUS with text readings.
    READING ASSIGNMENTS in SIXTH EDITION
    READING ASSIGNMENTS in SEVENTH EDITION

    For those purchasing the older Fifth Edition of Hergenhahn's text:
    CLASS SYLLABUS for HERGENHAHN FIFTH EDITION
    BUT please note some of the order of the readings has changed from that older syllabus!



    DISTRACTIONS:

    Stellarium is a free program that will simulate a planetarium on your computer so that you will always know what stars you're looking at and when something interesting will be appearing. It's quite addictive.

    Two short excerpts about James Gibson's Ecological Psychology: these are written by people who DON'T actually agree with Gibson and his ecological view, but who are describing him and his work fairly objectively. (While they're sort of skeptical, personally I'm not!) It's difficult to sum up the approach in brief; the linked passages are decent outsider views, but they're still incomplete and fail to appreciate some subtleties and philosophical implications. Ecological psychology is an approach to problems of perception and other aspects of psychology that is very different from conventional mainstream approaches. Instead of looking at the mind as a kind of computer involved in the processing of information (which is what mainstream psychology assumes), it is concerned with how animals and people can directly detect information in the environment which will be sufficient to guide their actions. The phrase "directly detect" is why the approach is often referred to as "direct perception"; it implies that the information doesn't need to be processed at all, which is controversial to say the least, and which certainly flies in the face of many centuries of epistemology. (At the end of the excerpt is an example of what a conventional "INdirect perception" approach looks like, for comparison.) The term "ecological" refers to a view of the animal and its environment as an integrated and co-evolving whole, as opposed to the conventional approach which seems to view the animal as an arbitrary observer placed into an arbitrary context. The ecological approach was developed by James Gibson over the whole course of his career, and today the University of Connecticut is the world leader in promoting and pursuing this approach. In particular the Psychology Department's Center For The Ecological Study Of Perception And Action (CESPA) is dedicated to advancing research and theory in ecological psychology.

    Silicon Immortality: Downloading Consciousness Into Computers, by neuroscientist (and author) David Eagleman: an interesting thought, very popular among some scientists and philosophers and especially science fiction writers. But I think it's based on an incorrect premise from the start, and thus is nonsensical. I used to express my annoyance at this idea being taken seriously by simpy titling this link "AAAAAAARRRRRRRRRGGGGGGGGHHHHHHHH THIS STUFF NEVER ENDS."

    Edward Tolman is my favorite psychologist.

    Spiders and turkeys and bears: some pictures from backyards (mine, my brother's). You're free to exclaim "oh my!" after reading the album title. Just something to look at if you're bored.



    LINKS AND READINGS:

    Some relevant quotes, capturing some insights into the nature of science and psychology. (Not all of it is immediately relevant at the beginning of the course.) Here are the two standard quotes to start off the History And Systems Of Psychology course:

    Various schools of thought in psychology which co-existed in the beginning decades of the field's history, and the implications of this situation for a history of psychology course.

    Defining psychology as a natural science: four definitions of psychology; three important dates in the history of psychology; a timeline and four terms that are central to epistemology. (An expanded version of the epistemology section will be appear again later in the course.)
    • Language Development: here are a couple of brief passages scanned from Erika Hoff's text that describe the Chomskyan view of language acquisition, along with an orientation to the perspectives of empiricism and nativism.

    Some perspectives on the "fate" of Behavioristic Psychology:

    Excerpt from an interview with philosopher Daniel Dennett (from Jonathan Miller's collection titled States Of Mind) on the strategy of investigating the workings of the brain by probing the information processing capacities of the mind, rather than approaching it directly through neuroscience. Note that this difference in investigational strategies is referred to as top-down vs. bottom-up, but in a different sense than cognitive psychologists usually mean.

    Excerpt from Out Of Our Heads by Alva Noë: from an introductory chapter of a book by one of the few philosophers to question the seemingly fundamental notion that the mind is the product of the brain. It's important to understand that he doesn't promote any supernatural alternative, but rather argues that the basic concept needs to be reconsidered.

    Thomas Nagel's Precis of his book Mind And Cosmos in which he questions some very fundamental assumptions about the nature of the universe.

    Localization of Cortical Function slides, highlighting some conceptual milestones from Gall to Penfield

    Franz Joseph Gall and phrenology, along with some of his legitimate scientific achievements.

    Phineas Gage's story is related here, with admirable restraint, for those who want to read the details. But all the links make interesting browsing. Phineas's damaged skull and his tamping iron are displayed at the Warren Anatomical Museum of the Countway Library of Medicine at Harvard University; next time you're in the Boston area, stop in. The story of the daguerreotype image identified in July 2009 is recounted here.

    Wilder Penfield and the motor and somatosensory areas in the human brain: a brief excerpt from an Intro Psych textbook, provided as an optional reference since he's not covered in Hergenhahn.

    Some colorful pictures I use to highlight the power of colorful pictures of brains to give extra weight to scientific points:
    Here's a brain image with colors representing various levels of blood oxygen consumption during a task (which I could not identify, unfortunately). Next, a 3D brain image colored to represent average amounts of different types of brain tissue present in twelve adolescents with schizophrenia, all mapped onto the same abstract brain image -- that is, the pictured brain is not actually the brain of any individual. Note that these images aren't really "pictures" of the brain or its activity, but are more like graphs in the shape of brains. Part of the information conveyed in the graphs is of course the location of the activity, so it's an appropriate representation. Now look at a brain image with some added fanciful colors,which is not a graph at all, just some fun imagery. The departure from any data shows that colors can be arbitrary, limited only by the good taste of the illustrator. Here's a brain illustration with lobes identified by colors, pictured inside a head, and here's a similar brain without the head included. Since this shape could be used as a graph to represent the information in the first two pictures, it should be equally plausible that those brain activity levels could instead be mapped onto a completely different image, such as this Volkswagen Beetle with different internal components identified by colors. The math required to map brain activity onto this image instead of onto an image of a brain would presumably not be different in kind from the math used to map the activity onto an image of a brain. But if that brain activity were graphed in the shape of the Volkswagen, I think it would lose a lot of its rhetorical authority -- we'd be more aware we're looking at data, not viewing actual brain processes in any concrete sense. I'm not advocating using arbitrary patterns to represent data (and thereby obscuring important location information), I'm just emphasizing that there's an extra dimension of persuasiveness conveyed by the impression the brain image makes on us.

    Three abstracts on the possibly unwarranted persuasiveness of brain images:

    Take an MRI tour of a really great brain: from left to right, from front to back, from top to bottom.

    Irving Kirsch on 60 Minutes, 2/19/12: in this segment Kirsch (a UConn psychology professor from 1975 to 2004) discusses his research that suggests anti-depressant medications are no more effective than a placebo, i.e., they work through expectations and beliefs rather than chemical effects of the drugs. Other supportive or critical points of view are offered too. Here's a transcript if you're interested.

    Washington Post article on psychiatrist Daniel Amen: "Daniel Amen is the most popular psychiatrist in America. To most researchers and scientists, that's a very bad thing." It's basically a damning esposé that doesn't seem to have affected the guy's business very much. Daniel Amen advocates very strongly for his brain-scan-based approach to psychiatry, especially on PBS stations which often broadcast his programs (or really, infomercials) during fundraising periods because they're THAT popular with audiences hoping for objective advice and help with mental illness. I think Daniel Amen is a goofball and self-promoter more than a charlatan about SPECT (single positron emission computed tomography, a relatively outdated technology compared to MRI and others), since to be a charlatan he'd have to not believe it himself, and I think he does believe it. He's the Dr. Oz of psychiatry. The links I've provided are uniformly critical, as Amen already does a good enough job of promoting himself.

    Connectionism / Parallel Distributed Processing (PDP) / Neural Networks: a few milestones backward in time, showing that "new" is relative.

    Koole, S.L., Greenberg, J., & Pysczcynski, T. (2006). Introducing psychology to the science of the soul. Psychological Science, 15 (5), 212-216: This is a paper on finding meaning in life vs. the fear of death -- not very natural-science-oriented topics -- that nonetheless proposes future research directions involving cognitive neuroscience and PDP models (see p. 215); this makes the point that these are truly pervasive features of modern cognitive psychology. But I've only posted the paper here for those of you who may be interested.

    Deep neural networks are more accurate than humans at detecting sexual orientation from facial images, by Michal Kosinski and Yilun Wang (in pre-publication form): An interesting and disturbing recent development in Parallel Distributed Processing / Neural Network / Connectionist computer modeling -- a PDP network is able to detect sexual orientation purely from facial features. Of course the success rate depends on how you measure success, and the photos used in the study were taken from a web site where people self-identify as gay or straight and presumably present themselves in the most appealing way they can (grooming, pose, angle of photo, etc.) which might make theses photos convey things that real life faces normally don't. The authors caution about the potential for abuse of such a technology, especially in terms of privacy issues as well as misinterpretation and oversimplification.

    Kant proposed three reasons why there could not be a science of the mind, but several 19th Century physiologists and early psychologists met each of those objections. For instance, Kant claimed psychology was impossible because:
    1. The mind wasn't a physical entity -- but then Helmholtz measured the speed of the nerve impulse and other aspects of the physical basis of thought.
    2. The mind could not be objectively observed since introspection was the only possible method and would always reveal the mind in the process of introspecting itself -- but then Donders invented the reaction time methodology which provided objective measures of the functioning of other minds.
    3. The mind could not be subjected to mathematical analysis -- but then Weber and Fechner came up with precise mathematical expressions that related the intensity of a physical stimulus (like light) to the magnitude of the subjective sensory impression it produced (perceived brightness).

    PowerPoint slides on Early Psychology: some of psychology's 19th century precursors, followed by Wundt and voluntarism, Titchener and structuralism, the imageless thought debate with Kulpe, and Watson's turn to behaviorism -- all in very brief overview.

    Donders's reaction time methodology as used in PET brain imaging research: an excerpt from Russell Poldrack's 2018 book The New Mind Readers describing Donders's legacy for cognitive neuroscience; see mainly pp. 31-32, with the rest getting into more specific findings as examples. Positron Emission Tomography (PET) was a major precursor to today's dominant technology, functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI).

    John Watson and Rosalie Rayner's original paper on Little Albert: Judge for yourself whether this paper is a well-controlled and judiciously interpreted study deserving of the attention it's gotten for the better part of a century. Is ONE subject enough? Are they doing classical or operant conditioning? Is Albert's crying really attributable uniquely to that rat and the noise associated with it?

    Quotes from William James on various topics

    Slides covering four definitions of Psychology in PDF format and in PowerPoint format: nearly identical to the "Defining psychology as a natural science" link above for definitions 1 & 2 but considerably expanded for definitions 3 & 4.

    Crimson by Josip Novakovich is a moving and disturbing short story that features this intriguing passage -- an insight that is certainly knowledge of a sort, but not scientific knowledge: "Milan thought how strange it was that he should be held responsible for the past, three years ago, when he was conscripted and enslaved--when he wasn't even himself. 'We all have multiple personalities,' he said. 'One of us is the past, and another the future, and there's no present me. We are vacant right now--spaces through which the past and the future disagree.'" Knowledge comes in many varieties, and whether it's scientific, or poetic, or spiritual, or intuitive, or common-sense, etc., all of these are valid ways of knowing the world for different purposes and contexts. Science is not the be-all-and-end-all of knowledge, or the ultimate arbiter of what is real for all purposes, but is a specially constructed tool for uncovering certain kinds of facts about nature using certain agreed-upon types of evidence. If other kinds of facts are not amenable to that sort of investigation, it doesn't necessarily make them any less real.

    Here are a couple of ways to get things really wrong, in viewing the relationship between science and religion:

    Notes On The Mind-Body Problem: I've summarized some information from Paul Churchland's Matter And Consciousness (Revised edition), and while there's a little more detail here than you need, it's better than the mere 1.5 pages (pp. 17-19) offered in Hergenhahn's Chapter 1. I've also added an outline and an explanation of exactly how Churchland's terminology differs from Hergenhahn's. (Depending on your browser, my nice numbered organization of the various positions might be very messed up, but you'll get the point.)

    Chapter 2 on "The Mind-Body Problem" from Paul Churchland's Matter And Consciousness (Revised edition, 1988): In case you're very interested in these issues and want to read much more detailed descriptions and arguments than I've provided, this was my original source. The perspective called "Eliminative Materialism" is Churchland's own favorite, but I find it unpersuasive and not historically useful for our purposes so I omitted it from my outline. There's a notice about not disseminating this material but I figure I'm not posting it, only linking to someone else's post. That seems okay. But if you're interested, get the whole book, there's a lot more in it than just the mind-body problem.

    Excerpt from 50 Great Myths of Popular Psychology: Shattering Widespread Misconceptions about Human Behavior by Scott O. Lilienfeld, Steven Jay Lynn, John Ruscio, and Barry L. Beyerstein (2009): this is a really good book that I'd strongly recommend to anyone interested in psychology. The publisher has made Chapter 1 available on their website; see especially "Myth #2" on pp. 25-28 for the discussion of brain lateralization (right vs. left hemispheres) and split-brain patients, related to Roger Sperry's position on the mind-body problem which we identify as interactionist property dualism. The rest of the chapter is quite interesting too, as you'll find when you can't stop reading it.

    Where Am I?: a philosophical article (by Daniel Dennett) in the form of a short story, that highlights the links between the mind-body problem and the idea of personal identity.

    A brief statement of the strangeness of quantum physics without a lot of explanation, in case you just want to see what's so strange.

    The double-slit experiment in quantum physics: painful as it is for me to admit, this clip from the terrible terrible stupid deceptive pseudo-documentary "Down The Rabbit Hole" (sequel to the equally stupid "What The Bleep Do We Know")... is actually a pretty good self-contained illustration of the famous paradoxical demonstration. Don't watch the rest of either of those movies, but do watch these five minutes.

    Interjections, an animated clip from the Schoolhouse Rock series: when you're happy or sad or frightened or mad or excited or glad, those words may just be shorthand for the relation between your situation and your observable behaviors. (Here's the same song starting at the beginning.)

    Two excerpts from Stephen Pinker's book How The Mind Works in which the Computational Theory of Mind is nicely described. Note that at one point, Pinker claims this theory is different from "the despised computer metaphor," which he suggests is the idea that the brain is exactly like a digital computer. But no one actually thinks that, and when people "despise" the computer metaphor, it actually is Pinker's own view that they mean. Pinker just wants to make it seem that no one could possibly have any reasonable objection to what he's saying. This is typical of Pinker, and part of the reason many people (including me) can't stand a lot of what he writes; another believer in the Computational Theory of Mind, the famous philosopher Jerry Fodor, found enough to disagree with in this book that he wrote his own book as a response, titled The Mind Doesn't Work That Way. These pages are an interesting read nonetheless.

    ENIAC article in Wikipedia: a description of the Electronic Numerical Integrator and Computer, commonly (though perhaps controversially) considered to be the first electronic computer. The general History Of Computing Hardware article is pretty interesting too, if you just want to see a long chronology of the ancestors of your PC.

    The Humans Are Dead by Flight Of The Conchords, in which robots reflect on the fate of the humans they have vanquished and replaced. It's from the first episode of their HBO show where they film a video for the song with no budget for special effects.

    A statue passes a dog's Turing test: watch as this dog finds the seated figure to be a passable imitation of a human, and tries to interact with it accordingly.

    Here are a few trenchant observations from philosopher John Searle about how dualism seems to remain viable not due to its inherent virtues, but due to materialism's failures to offer a convincing alternative. Note that his famous "Chinese Room" thought experiment is a demonstration of the computer metaphor's failure to account for knowledge and consciousness, and as such is therefore a critique of the materialist view called functionalism.

    PowerPoint slides on Philosophy of Science in text format

    Science Doesn't Work the Way You Might Think -- Not even for Einstein, by Thomas Levenson in The Atlantic: this short article describes the discovery and un-discovery of the planet Vulcan (nearer to the Sun than Mercury) which was believed to be a planet for the second half of the 19th century, due to the implications of Newtownian physics and people's inclination to observe what their theories tell them they should observe -- which is of course a Kuhnian notion.

    Randy Hickey from My Name Is Earl distills the essence of Kuhn's concept of the paradigm as disciplinary matrix.

    A somewhat racy illustration of the effects of relative motion on different observers' perceptions of time in special relativity

    Here's a segment from the 3/30/15 PBS NewsHour (mainly the portion from about 2:15 to 5:30) featuring some psychology research on implicit racism, using the Implicit Associations Test, Eye Tracking, and Event Related Potentials measured from the brain on the surface of the scalp: watch the video or read the transcript. This is especially relevant to the topic of Kuhnian paradigms since it's a great example of how standard experimental methods like reaction time, eye tracking, and ERP measures can be used more generally as ways of getting at issues in (for instance) social psychology, when none of them were developed with that specific goal in mind. A productive method can be very generally applied within a field.

    A discussion of the basis of trust in science: In this March 25, 2012 segment from the MSNBC program "UP with Chris Hayes" (5:57 to 10:10 is the relevant part) several panelists address a paradox: science says that we should believe its claims based on evidence, but that evidence is usually inaccessible to non-scientists, which leaves them in a position of having to trust the scientists, which in turn leads to the question of how we decide whom to trust. The panel includes psychologist Stephen Pinker, science journalist Robert Wright, and evolutionary biologist / atheist insult comic Richard Dawkins. (Not to slight the two women -- I'm unfamiliar with them but their contributions are illuminating too.) The specific issue under discussion was the question of how private belief might enter into the public sphere in terms of politicians' attitudes about evolution or climate change, especially if they're influenced by private religious convictions. It also gets at the idea that maybe, as Wright says, if you want to supplant religious doctrine with a scientific perspective as in the case of evolution or climate change, maybe it's better not to mock and insult religious people as Dawkins explicitly does. It's not a definitive or even essential discussion by any means but if you're interested in seeing the rest of it, here's the first part.

    Historians Admit To Inventing Ancient Greeks: "We were young and trying to advance our careers, so we just started making things up: Homer, Aristotle, Socrates, Hippocrates, the lever and fulcrum, rhetoric, ethics, all the different kinds of columns -- everything... Way more stuff than any one civilization could have come up with, obviously."

    PowerPoint slides from Thales to Democritus in text format.

    Heisenberg on Heraclitus: a short paragraph in which one of the founders of quantum mechanics points out the analogy between Heraclitus's views and contemporary physics. From Werner Heisenberg (1958), Physics and Philosophy: The Revolution in Modern Science.

    Das Rad (The Wheel): a 2003 Oscar nominee for best animated short film; its relation to Heraclitus's philosophy will be fairly clear.

    A concise statement on Parmenides from W.K.C.Guthrie (1950), The Greek philosophers from Thales to Aristotle.

    Steve Martin reflects on philosophy and religion and other stuff, c. 1977.

    Some Pythagoras links, for those who would be Pythagoreans if they could:

    Shakespeare's famous lines from The Merchant Of Venice about the Pythagorean "music of the spheres", which is hidden from us while our souls are imprisoned in our earthly bodies.

    Hunting the Hidden Dimension, an episode of the PBS series NOVA that explores the mathematics and applications of fractals in a very accessible and interesting way. The segment from 8:02 to 10:10 is a nice capsule summary of how fractals describe nature differently than traditional mathematics.

    Arcadia, Tom Stoppard's play as described in Wikipedia. If you choose, you can read this entire synopsis including details of the plot, and it will not ruin the experience of reading the actual play -- which, I remind you, is quite a fast read and is available at libraries and could change how you look at the world. But if the synopsis bores you, don't let that put you off. You liked Shakespeare In Love, didn't you? Same author. Also Rosencrantz and Guildenstern Are Dead, and Travesties, mentioned above.

    A short excerpt from Jamie James's The Music Of The Spheres in which he describes the Pythagorean context for Beethoven's revolutionary musical achievement. Also of interest on the scientific heritage of Pythagoras's view of music:

    Greek Philosophy through Aristotle, from Thomas Leahey's textbook on the history of psychology. It's a little more sophisticated than Hergenhahn's treatment so it may make for interesting reading; you don't have to study from it though!

    Laughing Philosopher / Weeping Philosopher, by John Heath-Stubbs: Those are the traditional nicknames of Democritus and Heraclitus, referring to their respective attitudes toward their shared acknowledgement of the impermanence of the world. This poem is inspired by that difference, as described by Montaigne: "Democritus and Heraclitus were both philosophers; the former, finding our human circumstances so vain and ridiculous, never went out without a laughing or mocking look on his face: Heraclitus, feeling pity and compassion for these same circumstances of ours, wore an expression which was always sad, his eyes full of tears." (Montaigne, Essays, c. 1580)

    Socrates is portrayed here in an excerpt from Sailing the Wine-Dark Sea: Why the Greeks Matter by Thomas Cahill. In just a few fun pages you get a character sketch, an example of his method of questioning (and why it was annoying), some mention of the nature of Plato's rendering of Socrates, and a note on his appeal to early Christianity due to the similiarities between him and Jesus.

    My Day With The Sophists: Not really, but given the description of the Sophists I offer in class, this seems almost appropriate. What it really is, is my account of my day of jury duty back in September 2000, which I wrote down because a bunch of friends kept asking me to tell them about it. On re-reading it, I wonder whether I'm actually more of a sophist than the attorneys. (Please keep in mind what a nice person I am, so that you're not annoyed that I come across as so obnoxious, elitist, and generally snarky.) This needs a serious re-write, having originated as an off-the-cuff email message, but I'm just not inclined to do it -- so it remains a little embarrassing, but makes the point okay.

    PowerPoint slides from the Sophists to Aristotle in text format.

    Examples of Plato's idealism in psychology as described in class.

    Some excerpts from Plato's Dialogues: They're very readable even in this classic translation from the 19th century. "Meno" features the slave boy demonstrating that he knows the Pythagorean Theorem (this version with illustrations may be easier to follow); Book VI of "Republic" describes the Divided Line; Book VII contains the Allegory Of The Cave. The excerpt from "Symposium" is Plato's famous discussion of the nature of love.

    Ode On A Grecian Urn, by John Keats: This poem is a meditation on a very old Greek urn (a vase) decorated with pictures of life at the time it was made. It captures the Platonic theme of abstract eternal existence, of music and love for instance (note especially the second stanza), as well as Plato's notion of "The Good" (in the last few lines).

    The Wreck Of The John B is the 1958 popular recording of the folk song, performed by The Kingston Trio, and it does a decent job of using the Pythagorean basics to fill out a pleasant tune. Compare that to the much more famous recording by the Beach Boys called Sloop John B for a closer approach to a sort of Platonic ideal, in terms of building a richer, more exciting arrangement gradually, out of parts that are very well integrated. Notice the initial flute and bells, and the strange entry of the bass, and the later removal of the instruments to expose the vocals for a climactic phrase. It may sound dated because the equipment and techniques were cutting edge but it was 1966 after all. Hopefully you can hear past that to the underlying musical vision.

    The School Of Athens, a fresco in the Vatican by Renaissance painter Raphael. This link helpfully allows you to click on the figures and identify them, but you may find other sites that additionally explain the significance of the figures' poses and activities. For instance, you will note the central positions of Plato, gesturing toward the heavens and the realm of the abstract ideal forms, and Aristotle, gesturing toward the natural world surrounding us which reveals itself to the careful observer. Maybe more important, though, is that links are provided to the major works of many of these philosophers so if you're interested, you can just click away.

    "Who died and left Aristotle in charge of ethics?" From the TV show "The Good Place" which has a lot of good philosophy like this.

    This Too Shall Pass, a music video by OK Go which nicely illustrates Aristotle's concept of "efficient cause". There's also a live version that's worth watching (though I can't think of any obvious Aristotelian connection, if that's what you look for in a video.)

    B.F.Skinner lecture excerpt 1 and B.F.Skinner lecture excerpt 2 in which he explicitly denies that the cause of behavior is to be found in the immediately preceding events and instead lies in the consequences or following events.

    Excerpt from the film of The Name Of The Rose: I let this clip drag on too long so it's a large file, but once the bad monk catches on fire you can turn it off. Don't watch it if you plan to see the whole movie, or read the book.

    Excerpt from Spook: Science Tackles the Afterlife by Mary Roach: a few subtly hilarious pages that take us through the history of scientific thought about how the soul gets into the body, from Aristotle to Descartes -- including the story of how sperm and egg were discovered when the first microscopes were turned toward... well, you know. Anything that begins, "There's a very good chance you underestimate the historic import of the sea urchin" is clearly right up my alley, and hopefully yours too. (Sorry the excerpt just leaves off... If you find this selection fun, don't go get the book -- instead, first read her debut Stiff about things that are done with human cadavers, which is high on my list of the most interesting and funniest things I've ever read.)

    PowerPoint slides on epistemology: These are slightly more current than the Epistemology web page that summarizes them. The web page version, though, may be a useful reference for some of the technical terms that come up in lecture, as well as for the timeline along the top. NOTE: the "Rationalism vs. Empiricism" section lists five questions that distinguish the two approaches, and the last three of those are not specifically intended for PSYC 3100 -- so if you find them mystifying you can ignore them. The last of them may describe some familiar comparisons though.

    Excerpt from Democracy In America (Book 2, Section 1, Chapter 1) by Alexis de Tocqueville: read just the first long paragraph -- in fact, just read through the point where Descartes is mentioned -- and you can see the claim that Descartes's method is obvious to us because it's been part of our basic outlook from the time the U.S. was settled. (If you're unfamiliar with Tocqueville and his 1835/1840 book, take a moment to acquaint yourself with it here, since it's one of the most influential books ever written about the United States. His predictions about our future were uncanny, including the Civil War over slavery, the US and Russia becoming opposing superpowers, and descriptions of the political and social characteristics of the country that are perfectly applicable today.)

    Three ways Hume was really influential in psychology were:
    1. Hume said simple ideas could be built up into complex ideas through some simple laws of association, instead of depending on the mind playing an active role with its unexplained homunculus abilities. PDP models of information processing are based on applying a few generic rules to a large collection of simple processing units instead of depending on a program that executes an explicit set of instructions.
    2. Hume said all knowledge could be reduced to either matters of fact or observation (synthetic statements) or matters of the relations among ideas (analytic statements), and anything else didn't count as knowledge at all. Logical positivism was the philosophy of science in the 20th century that took that as its starting point and said science was the model for all knowledge; it was the most influential philosophy underlying behaviorism, affecting the kinds of theories that were developed and favoring the neglect of any role for the mind.
    3. Hume said that cause and effect couldn't be perceived with the senses and were merely the result of observing repeated contiguity between successive events, along with the unjustifiable assumption that the future will be like the past; therefore the best attitude toward science would be to stick to cataloging the observations and their typical sequences without proposing any underlying causal explanations of why things happen. The most influential behaviorist, B.F. Skinner, adopted much the same attitude by arguing against complicated theorizing in terms of physiology or cognition or other unobservable variables, and instead just cataloging the observed relationships between stimuli, responses, and reinforcements and punishments.

    David Hume on causation, from An Enquiry Concerning Human Understanding (1748) -- in case you want to see where questions about causality all began. (See also the famous conclusion.)

    Outline of Logical Positivism: This is a very brief sketch of the philosophy of science known as Logical Positivism, which was very influential in the development of psychology in the first half of the 20th century. Note the concluding segment, an excerpt from the first English-language manifesto of logical positivism, in which Hume's influence is explicitly acknowledged. The diagram of the logical positivist view of science cited above may be useful to note here as well.

    Recording of brief lecture on epistemology (46 minutes, 21MB) to supplement readings on Descartes, Locke, Berkeley, Hume, and Kant


    A student complained to Joseph Campbell about the volume of reading in his class: "How do you expect me to complete all this in a week?" He just laughed and said, "I'm astonished you tried. You have the rest of your life to do the reading."

    Demos from my two bands - kind of amateur-ish, but enthusiastic. In both bands I'm on piano, keyboards, and organ, and my brother is the drummer.


    If you're wondering about classes being canceled due to weather, see http://alert.uconn.edu or call 486-3768.