PSYC 1100 sec 35-47, Fall 2019: EXAM 1 STUDY GUIDE
EXAM 1: FRIDAY 10/11/19 3:35PM, SCHN 151;
#2 LEAD PENCIL REQUIRED!
REVIEW SESSION: Wednesday 10/9/19 6:15-7:30-ish PM, MCHU 102;
bring questions to have answered, etc.
NOTE: Although the topic of split-brain patients IS included in the review information below, it will NOT appear on the first exam. Don't study it -- it's only there in case we get to it, and also so it's available for the second exam.
The first exam will have 50 multiple choice questions
drawn from lecture and textbook material and will be do-able
in one hour (or less). Don't expect the multiple choice format to mean
you'll just be looking through a list of alternatives trying
to recognize some familiar information. The emphasis
throughout will not be merely on rote memory for facts and
definitions, but rather on conceptual understanding of the
material. For example, unfamiliar questions related to the lecture
content may be included, requiring an application of the
knowledge you have gained so far. But not in a scary
way. You will benefit from studying as if this were partly an essay exam,
even though there will be only multiple choice questions on it!
Check the web page for PSYC 1100 sec 35-47 at
http://media.pluto.psy.uconn.edu/psyc1100.html, and from
there you can get to a rough outline of the course material.
The outline
is just the text of my PowerPoint slides -- it's by no means an
exhaustive account of all the course material, and it doesn't
contain all the detail you will need, but it does serve as an
outline of the material and a reminder of some key concepts
to know. You can also find my study tips page (quite long, pretty useful).
Sample exams: Practice exam-taking using old exam questions!
PRACTICE EXAM QUESTIONS: Here are some questions from the exams of another professor who taught this course similarly to the way I do. I've taken questions from two of his exams and arranged them by topic for my class, so ignore the numbering of the items (and the chapter numbers, for that matter -- older edition of the textbook!). I strongly recommend not looking at the answer key until you've tried to discover the answers for yourself first.
ONLY the NEUROPSYCHOLOGY QUESTIONS are relevant for Exam 1! Note that many, if not most, of the neuropsychology questions are about topics covered in the reading but not yet covered in lecture, due to that professor's different emphasis on brain region topics and lack of coverage of neurons, action potentials, neurotransmitters, etc. So don't be discouraged if they're as yet unfamiliar; maybe hold off on them till after they've been addressed in lecture. Though you can probably find answers in the textbook if you look.
ANSWERS TO THE PRACTICE EXAM QUESTIONS. This will only help you if you
first make every effort to answer the questions on your own, using the
text and your notes. Ignore the advice to "look at the other exams too," because those exams are no longer available!
Lecture vs. text:
The lecture material is primary; use the text as a resource
to support and elaborate the lecture topics.
If something is covered in lecture, you will be responsible
for the lecture coverage and ALL of the required text
coverage of that topic as listed on the syllabus, unless I
specifically tell you to omit certain pages (see below).
If something is NOT covered in lecture, you will NOT be
responsible for the text coverage of that topic, unless I
specifically tell you to study certain pages (see below).
Page numbers for all required reading are listed on the
syllabus.
The Gleitman text isn't just a really long story that you
read straight through repeatedly. You should read all the assigned
material, probably at least twice. But then also use the
index to find answers to particular questions that come up in
your notes, your reading, or the old exams. Use the summary
points at the end of the chapters, and the contents at the
beginning of chapters, to help you identify what's there and
how it's organized.
What to study:
Start with the readings listed on the syllabus, and then here
are my comments:
Introductory material not found in the textbook is in a few links on the web page. One ("Some Introductory notes") lists the three major dates in psychology's history and the four definitions of what psychology is about (you can skip the parts about epistemology for this exam, though they mauy come up later in the course). Another is the "Brief overview of psychology's history", providing an outline of the field's history and the reasons for some of the changes it has undergone (start with Wundt on p. 9). Finally there are the PowerPoint slides elaborating on the third and fourth definitions.
NERVOUS SYSTEM OUTLINE:
an outline of what to know about the nervous system for this course.
Here's a pdf version, in case the formatting is messed up
in the Word version.
And here's a web page version.
Summary of the
experiment on patients with either a damaged amygdala or hippocampus that was mentioned in class, but is not in the textbook.
Basic terms in classical conditioning (US, UR, CS, CR), also included as part of the text of the PowerPoint slides to go with the reading at the beginning of the Learning chapter (pp. 195-201).
Basic description of
what phrenology was and what other legitimate discoveries can be attributed to Franz Joseph Gall.
Neural Bases of Behavior. Here's what the syllabus says:
SEVENTH EDITION: read all of Ch.3, except you can SKIP pp. 83-85, 97-99, and 112. Then also READ pp. 46-49 and 56-57 as well as figure 2.1 on p. 48 to cover the hypothalamus and autonomic nervous system; and finally pp. 302-308 on the pre-frontal cortex and consciousness, including the phenomenon of "blindsight".
EIGHTH EDITION: read all of Ch. 3 (note figure 3.26 on p. 114 on the autonomic nervous system), except you can SKIP pp. 103-105 ("Communication Through The Bloodstream") and 108-112 (from "Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Studies" through "The Power of Combining Techniques" -- good material but it won't be on the exam). Then also READ these pages from other chapters: for more on the ANS read pp. 464-466 ("Thermoregulation") and 469 ("Hypothalamic Control Centers") and 474-475 ("The Fight Or Flight Response"); and finally pp. 223-224 on the phenomenon of "blindsight".
Topics in the text not explicitly covered in lecture -- be sure to read: "Plasticity" pp. 113-115 in SEVENTH edition; pp. 125-127 in EIGHTH edition.
Brief outline:
Structure of the neuron
Communication between neurons (the action potential and neurotransmitters)
Divisions of the nervous system (see handout)
The brain from bottom to top (hindbrain, midbrain, forebrain
- see handout)
The cortex lobe by lobe (frontal, parietal, occipital, temporal)
Primary projection areas (know motor, somatosensory, visual, auditory locations and functions)
Non-primary (or "association") areas (know locations and functions of pre-frontal cortex, Broca's and Wernicke's areas)
Front-Back and Left-Right distinctions in the brain
Disorders resulting from lesions to non-primary areas:
- pre-frontal lesions, apraxia, agnosia, neglect, expressive/Broca's/non-fluent aphasia, receptive/Wernicke's/fluent aphasia (Gleitman uses the "non-fluent" and "fluent" terminology.)
- know name of syndrome, symptoms, and probable location of
damage (what lobe and/or hemisphere)
Split-brain studies: corpus callosum, abilities of each
hemisphere, contralateral connections from visual field to each hemisphere,
contralateral control of hands
- to understand and interpret experiments, know:
which side of visual field info is shown to;
which hemisphere gets the info; what abilities does that
hemisphere have; which hand does it control
Learning: The Changing Organism's Adaptation to the
Changing World. Just read Ch.6 pp. 195-201 in the SEVENTH edition (pp. 259-265 in the EIGHTH edition), stopping before the section titled "Extinction". But "second order conditioning" will not be on this first exam.
Names to know (roughly in order of appearance in this
course):
Wilhelm Wundt (founder of psychology as an experimental science, 1879), John Watson (proposed psychology should be the science of behavior, 1913), Ulric Neisser (coined the term "cognitive psychology", 1967), Rene Descartes, Charles Sherrington, Franz Joseph Gall, Wilder Penfield, Ivan Pavlov
Brief note on the processes of inhibition and
excitation:
Inhibition and Excitation are "opponent processes", like
opposing forces (inhibition is the negative force, and
excitation is the positive force), appearing in many guises
throughout the nervous system and in behavior. Think of them
as nature's general approach to solving a host of different
problems of nervous system design. We've encountered them a
few times already (and there will be more!):
in neural communication --
An excitatory connection means the neurotransmitter makes the
postsynaptic cell more likely to fire, by activating
receptor molecules that allow more positive Na+ ions inside
(i.e., the -70mV inside becomes more positive, and therefore
closer to the -55 mV threshold for the action potential). An
inhibitory connection means the neurotransmitter makes the
postsynaptic cell less likely to fire (i.e., the -70mV
inside becomes more negative, and therefore farther from the
-55 mV threshold for the action potential), by activating
receptor molecules that allow more negative Cl- ions inside
(technically, they also push positive K+ ions outside).
Whether a particular neuron sends out an action potential
depends on how all the excitatory and inhibitory connections
add up; if all the inhibition cancels out all the excitation
there is no action potential, but if the excitation is strong
enough, there is one.
in the autonomic nervous system --
Excitation of the sympathetic nervous system serves an
activating function, arousing the organism as in preparedness
for emergency (e.g., heart rate is accelerated, digestion and
sexual functioning are inhibited). Excitation of the
parasympathetic nervous system serves vegetative or
resting functions (e.g., heart beats at a relaxed pace,
stomach secretes digestive enzymes, sexual functions are
performable). These two systems act against each other, in
an inhibitory relationship -- that is, functions that are
excited by one system are inhibited by the other, as
demonstrated in the preceding examples. In this case you
don't say that one division (say, the sympathetic) is
inhibitory and the other excitatory -- you simply note that
their effects work in opposite directions, or that they
inhibit each other. There is a brief discussion of these
processes on pp. 46-49 and 56-57, with an excellent diagram
(Fig. 2.1) on p. 48 in Gleitman.
in cases of disinhibition --
Disinhibition refers to the fact that we can sometimes
observe the effects of inhibition by seeing what happens when
that inhibition is removed (hence "disinhibition").
It's been addressed in lecture in the case of the praying
mantis's copulatory reflex : the male praying
mantis performs copulatory movements reflexively and even more vigorously
after the higher nerve centers responsible for inhibiting that reflex
are removed, if the female bites the male's head off. And in the text,
there is Sherrington's work (p. 107) on spinal reflexes in dogs, which
depended on severing the
spinal cord from the brain in order to remove the brain's
inhibitory effects on the reflexes being studied, so that the
dog's "will" wouldn't exert an influence. (Along those
lines, remember we mentioned how a human's withdrawal reflex
might be inhibited by the higher-level knowledge that
although a heated dish of food feels painfully hot while we
carry it, we should avoid letting go of it -- as long as it's
not causing injury -- until we reach the table.)
Disinhibition also comes up indirectly: for instance, one effect
of alcohol is to
temporarily damage the higher brain areas, resulting in a
loss of inhibition of impulsive behaviors; and
one way of interpreting the behavior of Phineas Gage after
his accident is as a loss of inhibition of impulsive
behaviors due to the lack of social constraints normally
imposed through the action of his missing pre-frontal brain
areas.
in the case of split-brain patients --
Anecdotally, for a brief period of time after
surgical severing of the corpus callosum, odd effects of
apparent competition between the two hemispheres arise in a
few cases. One hand may reach for a dress while the other
picks out a different one; one hand may stock cans on a shelf
while the other takes them down. Though there is no real
theory to explain this and the effects are rare, one
interpretation is that even normal behavior (i.e., of
non-split-brain subjects like us) depends on the outcome of
opposing actions of the two hemispheres, such that one
inhibits the other and the outcome in observable behavior is
the result of the opposing forces.
General recommendations about studying:
I would make two recommendations about studying for all the exams in this class (and possibly in other classes):
1) First, a common experience is for students to have read the textbook, web links, and their notes, and feel quite secure that they understand everything -- but then not see that understanding reflected in their exam scores. Consider why that might be: looking at the material and having a feeling of understanding is a rather passive way to confirm your knowledge. I too can look at the text, web links, and notes, and feel like I understand them all, yet let's be honest, my understanding is probably quite a bit deeper than yours despite the similarity of the feeling. What you should aim for is not just recognition and sensibleness, but real familiarity and comfort, where you feel like you could actually explain the topic clearly to a fellow student who had a question about it, or even to someone who had no knowledge of it (e.g. a parent or roommate). Not that you literally need to deliver practice lectures to an audience, but maybe that's something to try to imagine, to see where your gaps and shortcomings might be.
2) Second, the multiple choice format often leads students to expect a kind of recognition test where terms are matched up with definition a, b, c, or d, or maybe a concept is described as being about a, b, c, or d. But multiple choice questions (mine included) can require you to think hard about comparisons or contrasts between perspectives, or ways that one idea implies or is linked to another, or applications of the topics to particular situations. For this reason, I find flashcards and the like to be of limited use, maybe good for memorizing what "myelin" is for or what the threshold for an action potential is, but flashcards tend to focus on isolated pieces of information rather than how concepts are related to one another.
It makes sense, for example, to know not just what "inhibition" means but also what forms it might take (disinhibition, reciprocal inhibition), various ways the concept appears in the nervous system and in behavior, examples, etc.
These are all things that are addressed explicitly in class, so I'm not implying that you need to creatively come up with these explanations; just don't assume that a superficial memory of a term and what it means will suffice. Sometimes students ask me if they just need to know the major points of what we covered, and unfortunately the answer is, no, that's not enough: you need to know the details too.
For these two reasons I usually suggest that the way to think about my exams is to pretend you're studying for an essay exam rather than a multiple choice exam. That way you realize you don't only need to know what the concepts mean, but you also have to be able to link them together and understand why and how they're connected. And you want to know this at a level where you'd be able to produce such an explanation in an essay, because even though you don't have to actually write it, that type of preparation will allow you to make the connections that the questions ask you to make. Students have sometimes said they don't think my questions are too difficult, really -- just that they make you think through them to figure them out instead of instantly answering or not answering correctly. That's exactly my intention.