SOME STUDY TIPS FOR PSYC 132: 72, Spring 2001

Here I've tried to write down everything I would ever tell someone who wanted advice about studying or how to do better in class. It's pretty long (maybe nine pages) but I think it's all good advice that would apply to any class in college. Browse at your leisure and see if it's worthwhile. Let me know if you think of any questions I didn't address, or any suggestions you think would be better than mine.

CONTENTS:

GENERAL STRATEGIES

  • 1) Don't bother highlighting the text (unless it really works for you)...
  • 2) Instead of highlighting (my personal strategy)...
  • 3) The advantages of this "headlining" strategy, as I see them...
  • 4) If you plan on taking notes on the assigned reading rather than highlighting...
  • 5) Timing of the reading with the lecture...
  • 6) To read the text...
  • 7) Read at the level of the page / paragraph / sentence...
  • 8) Take notes in class...
  • 9) Learning isn't just memorizing facts...
  • 10) What psychology tells you about studying...
    WEB PAGE
    OVERHEADS FOR THE CLASS SO FAR
    SOME OLD 132 EXAMS ON ELECTRONIC COURSE RESERVE
    EXAM FORMAT
    MULTIPLE CHOICE QUESTIONS
    WHAT TO STUDY

    GENERAL STRATEGIES

    1) Don't bother highlighting the text (unless it really works for you)...

    In my experience, highlighting passages in a textbook is almost always a waste. Every other sentence ends up yellow, or underlined, or with an asterisk next to it. Even if you're conservative with the highlighter, you still have to later figure out why you chose one sentence over another as the key point to know. If you think the reason will be obvious, try reading a used copy of a textbook that someone else has highlighted. Bet it doesn't help you much! In fact, it's mainly distracting, which should make you resolve not to do it to your books if you plan on selling them back at the end of the course. (Although you definitely pay enough for them to do whatever you want with them before you sell them back!)

    2) Instead of highlighting... (my personal strategy)

    When I read a text or a journal article or anything I'm trying to learn from, I write in the margin, lightly in pencil and in tiny print, a brief "headline" of a couple of words or a short phrase every paragraph or two (depending on the density of the information presented). If a paragraph is really long, I may also draw a bracket next to an important couple of lines, so I know which part the headline refers to. What I end up with is a running index alongside each page telling me where to find the bits I thought were important, and possibly the headline will give me a summary in a couple of words of what the accompanying text says. Looking back at those pages later I can immediately see what's covered there, and know if I have to read it again (if it's not familiar yet). The headline will also tell me what to expect when I read that paragraph, even without reading the preceding paragraphs again for context.

    3) The advantages of this "headlining" strategy, as I see them:

    First, just as with a newspaper headline, generating a headline for a paragraph margin takes active effort to recognize what's important in the text and come up with a brief description that indicates the substance of it. That kind of active consideration of the material will help you remember it. Highlighting, on the other hand, is usually pretty passive; the evaluation of the material goes no deeper than thinking "this sentence looks important, guess I'll make it yellow".

    Second, even if you try to be as active a highlighter as possible, thinking hard about what's important, you still end up reducing the whole (the paragraph) to a few of its elements (phrases or definitions that you try to make into proxies for the full text). But those sentences within a paragraph weren't meant to summarize it; they all contribute to the whole. Headlining gets you to synthesize (put together) information, rather than reduce it (strip it down to less than it was).

    Third, writing headlines in light pencil instead of highlighting in garish yellow marker allows you to erase and revise your headlines if you go back later and can't figure out what you meant, or if you decide the point is something other than what you first thought. If you used highlighting marker, your only option for revision would be to highlight even more text, or start in with a new color, or use asterisks for emphasis, until you have a well illustrated but nearly unreadable page of text left.

    The reason I write "in tiny print" is because I just like to write really tiny ... but it does keep the margins from overwhelming the text, too.

    4) If you plan on taking notes on the assigned reading rather than highlighting...

    Try to follow a similar strategy to the above. Don't just copy phrases from the text; instead, read the text, then write down what it's about (headlines) in an organized way, paragraph by paragraph, followed by details from those paragraphs. And definitely do not neglect the text in favor of your notes after that. On the contrary, you might be better off TAKING A NEW SET OF NOTES each time you look at the text, and NEVER EVEN READING THE NOTES YOU WRITE! If you digest and paraphrase the material several times, you'll probably have done the hard part already; your notes will just remind you of what you've learned. (It's good to have that reminder, of course.) In an ideal world, this is how everyone would read their texts; but given that people take 4 or 5 classes each semester, it's a little unlikely. Something to aim for, anyway.

    5) Timing of the reading with the lecture...

    You have to do the reading before the exam; whatever else you do is up to you. I STRONGLY recommend that you read about a topic just before or just after we cover that topic in class -- whichever works better for you. If you don't understand the text easily, listen to the lecture first and then go read. If you'd rather have some familiarity with the topic before class, have the chapter read ahead of time. It's up to you. Doing them close together, in either order, will be best.

    The syllabus lists all the pages you must read from each chapter, but it doesn't say in what order the lectures will treat that material. For instance, you probably noticed right away that the neuron came before the brain in lecture, but in the text the brain is treated first, then the neuron. When trying to locate a topic from class, you can flip through the chapter (which will help you find your way around it) or just go to the index, or table of contents.

    6) To read the text...

    Don't think you can read a textbook like you read a novel, or even a general nonfiction book. It's not a narrative, and is not meant to be read one time through from beginning to end, at which point everything will be clear to you. Rather, a great deal of information has been brought together, summarized, and organized as well as possible -- which still demands that you read slowly and carefully. You should always keep in mind what you're being told, and why, and how it relates to the preceding and following material. In other words, since the text has to impose organization on a wider range of topics than other types of reading usually cover, you have to make the effort to be a more organized reader and keep the author's organization in mind all the time.

    If you want to be really explicit about the organization (which can only help you), look at the hierarchy of topics and headings first, before you even read the chapter. The first page of each chapter in the Gleitman et al text lists all the sections you'll come across (e.g., on p. 14 of chapter 2 [sorry, I'm referring to an earlier editon of the text - I'll update this! The organization is very similar though] you see listed "THE CORTEX, p. 28") and one level of headings under that (e.g., Localization Of Function In The Cortex; Projection Areas). Looking in the chapter you find there are sub-headings under Projection Areas (primary motor area,... disorders of perception and attention,... etc.) and still another level under disorders (you find agnosias, neglect syndrome, Gerstmann syndrome in italics). It would be useful to write out an entire outline based on these headings, perhaps including any vocabulary terms in bold print at the lowest level, BEFORE you read the chapter. (There's a new heading every three paragraphs or so throughout!) With this outline in hand, you will have a much better feel for where you are, why you're reading what you're reading, and what it's connected to. Obviously, reading 50 pages of text like this is a lot more work than reading 50 pages of, say, a magazine. It's like the difference between watching an hour-long college lecture and watching an hour-long show on the Discovery channel.

    When you're done reading, it couldn't hurt to immediately flip to the last pages of the chapter and read the summary points covering what you've just read. If you've lost track of the point of it all, that should help remind you again.

    7) Read at the level of the page / paragraph / sentence...

    You can't really read textbooks by the chapter, as I said, or often even by the section; you have to read, roughly, by the page. This means concentrating hard on what's right in front of you, rather than having your goal be to cover a certain number of pages in a night. (That's also why you need to be very aware of the overarching organization as you study each particular topic closely.) One key to understanding the reading is not to move on to the next thing till you understand what you're reading at the moment. Lots of people are actually unaware of whether they understand something as they read it -- they just read it and move on. Be aware!

    Since a page of a book isn't inherently organized -- it's just the way a given number of words and pictures came out in the type-setting, after all -- the units you should really focus on for comprehension are the headings on the page, and more specifically, the content of those headings: paragraphs. Paragraphs aren't random bunches of sentences. It takes a certain number of sentences to make a point; paragraphs are the chunks of discourse, the points that are being made. In other kinds of reading you may browse through them, maybe checking the first sentence or sampling a couple of words to judge whether you feel like paying attention to what's inside them. In reading a textbook, you have to know what every one is about. That's why when I write my headlines in the margin (see above) I emphasize paragraphs, not whole sections and not individual sentences.

    It's also important, eventually, to read at the level of the individual sentence, since quite a lot of information may be packed into even one sentence, and fine distinctions can hinge on the interpretation of a particular turn of phrase. The more advanced your level of reading becomes (upper division courses, graduate school, professional journals, etc.), the more you'll want to respond to the implications of nearly every sentence. This is usually not an issue with introductory texts, which are written to be as unambiguous and straightforward as possible. Still, here's an example from an intro psych textbook I have (Kassin, Psychology (2E), 1998, p. 546) which makes the following statement:

    "[S]tudies reveal that women are more sensitive than men to how others are feeling -- a skill often referred to as 'female intuition.' Some researchers speculate that this advantage is rooted in the left-dominant female brain."

    Does this say that the female brain IS left-dominant (implying perhaps that men are right-dominant) and that the skill may be rooted there? Or does it say that some SPECULATE that the female brain is left-dominant, and furthermore, if it is, the skill may be rooted there? If you're planning on accepting that text as the authority, you'll want to be sure of what the author intends to tell you. (Most researchers in the field actually find it misleading to speak of a "dominant" hemisphere; if anything, males are thought to have more brain functions focused in one or the other hemisphere, while females have more functions spread between both. But how are you supposed to know that, if you're just reading an intro text?)

    8) Take notes in class...

    Note-taking (not just note-reading) helps remembering. It requires an active effort to extract the important points being made and then organize them on paper, whereas just reading the notes is basically a poor substitute for reading the fuller coverage in the textbook. (The obvious exception is material covered in class but not in the book.) As I implied above, given a choice between being allowed to TAKE notes but not read them later, or NOT taking notes but getting to read someone else's, I would definitely choose the note-TAKING option. Luckily, in real life you get to do both.

    It's hard to decide what to write down; if it was easy, everyone would have perfect transcripts of the lectures in their notebooks. It's also hard to extract important points while writing what the instructor said a moment ago AND paying attention to what the instructor is currently saying, guessing at spellings you don't know, trying to write legibly enough that you can decode your notes later, and so on. But it's an important skill, hard as it is, and you get better at it by doing it.

    Most people try to copy everything on the overhead slides, and their classroom task becomes one of frantic mechanical copying with little attention paid to the talking. No instructor wants that -- otherwise they could just post the overheads and skip teaching. Do your best to write down EVERYTHING that's said, in an organized manner, and if you can't copy all of the overheads too, just use them to guide you in deciding which are the key things the instructor is saying. A practice that helps is to take notes on one page of your notebook and leave the facing page blank, to be filled in later with information and elaborations from the text, from the web page, or from discussion with the instructor. Then all that information will be indexed to what was presented in lecture.

    9) Learning isn't just memorizing facts...

    Knowing the vocabulary of your subject area is a key goal -- the one that must be met before much else can be accomplished. But it's not an end in itself. If all you know is definitions, you won't know much about brain function, or learning theory, or whatever. (Plus, you'll undoubtedly confuse terms with one another: "dendrite / terminal ending", "temporal / parietal", etc.) Most people go beyond that and learn facts, or propositions, involving those new terms ("the hypothalamus is involved in motivation; the amygdala is involved in emotion"). That's better but still doesn't go far enough.

    Understanding the structure that relates all those pieces of information is the ultimate goal (e.g., how the very old cerebellum and the very new frontal cortex are both involved in producing smooth skilled movements). The different concepts should feel different to you, based on the roles they play in the grander scheme. They're not just neutral marks on a page of text, which you just have to match up with the correct set of other marks. Students think that's what multiple choice tests are all about, but they're wrong for two reasons: 1) even very subtle relationships can be addressed in multiple choice questions, if the test writer is good at it, and 2) even if the test does only ask about isolated bits of information, the student who has studied the structure and relations among all the concepts will probably remember them more easily and accurately and thus perform better anyway. (Essay exams are almost always preferable to multiple choice exams when practical, because they test directly the more important aspects of learning. In the real world after college, there's nothing like a multiple choice test, but there are lots of things like essay tests -- even if they don't involve any writing at all!)

    Many people try to use flash cards in studying. That doesn't hurt for the definitions and facts, but unless you're pretty creative with them, it also leads to the biggest mistake students make in their studying: focusing on isolated definitions and propositions, and neglecting the structure of the subject. Instead (or in addition) try something like making a list of comparisons between classical and operant conditioning, or reproducing from memory (hopefully not just rote memory!) the handout on the components of the nervous system.

    10) What psychology tells you about studying...

    I'll mention a few terms from the study of human memory: massed vs. distributed practice, expanding rehearsal, elaborative rehearsal, and organization."Massed practice" in the case of studying means doing all the studying at once -- for hours at a time say, and for several days in a row. I'm not gonna tell you you won't learn that way, but it's the least efficient way. It corresponds to cramming the night before the exam, or two nights before, or three (even though it becomes less like cramming as you add more nights of studying). Many students who do poorly on exams after putting in a lot of time wonder what they could have done differently ("I started studying five days before the exam..."). They could try distributing the same amount of study time over a longer period. (Note that another reason to avoid cramming is that cramming is just hard! If you pinch all the required textbook pages between your fingers it may not look very thick, but don't be fooled -- there's way too much stuff to even look at it all if you wait till the last minute.)

    "Distributed practice" means taking the same amount of study time but spreading it out, so there are periods (probably days) of non-studying interspersed. Study psychology for a couple of hours one night, then something else the next night, then something else a third night, then back to psychology. The time spent away from the subject actually contributes to the consolidation of your knowledge of it, in some mysterious way, and you can learn it better with the same amount of studying. The problem students in the real world face with this strategy, of course, is that an impending exam is strong motivation for lots of intense studying, while a distant exam is hardly the spur people need to get on a regular studying schedule. Bottom line, you just have to recognize what's best for you and motivate yourself to pursue it. When the exam is a week away and you haven't studied yet (which happens frequently, unfortunately), you can distribute practice a little, but it's not as important as getting through all the material a bunch of times. I've actually said to students in that situation that one week of consistent, concentrated studying should work out okay, but I always recommend long-term studying. Either way, you should treat studying for exams as you would a full-time job -- it takes a lot of time, but (at least in the narrowest sense) it's why you're here.

    "Expanding rehearsal" is a form of distributed practice which seems to work especially well, and supposedly there is research support specifically for its effectiveness in the classroom, not just the lab. The idea is, successful remembering of information makes that information easier to remember in the future. The act of retrieving information from memory works like exercise that makes you better at it next time; or perhaps it's like tramping down a path through your brain that will be easier to follow in the future. Of course, if you're also using distributed practice, the space between your study sessions makes it less likely that you'll successfully recall what you studied last time. So expanding rehearsal goes like this: make each interval between sessions as long as it can be (distributed) while you can still recall the information from the previous session. Maybe consecutive days the first time, then two days later the next time, then three... Each time you sucessfully recall the studied information, you know it that much better: you can go longer and longer without seeing it and still recall it accurately. Your practice is distributed in time, but in such a way that you always are able to remember the stuff from the last time -- no forgetting what you read and doing it all over again from scratch (at least ideally). To read more about these strategies look at pp. 409-414 in Alan Baddeley's (1998) Human Memory: Theory and Practice, Revised Edition (Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon).

    "Elaborative rehearsal" is an old term in memory research, but it refers essentially to what I've tried to emphasize above: rather than just repeatedly viewing isolated bits of information, you should elaborate the material. Add to your knowledge of each concept by trying to place it in the context of experiences you've had, if possible, and in the context of other concepts in psychology. Try to see how the topics within a chapter are related to each other, and why Henry Gleitman and I would consider it important that you understand these particular topics. The more you can organize and link together various initially isolated concepts, the better you'll understand and remember them. And of course, the notion of "organization" is also what I've been pushing a lot here. If you can build up a network of interrelated concepts with a definite structure or organization for each of the subject areas we study, you'll have as firm a grasp as possible of the course content, and that will support your performance on the exams. Baddeley's book has lots on these principles, but your own text also describes elaborative rehearsal and organization in memory (see Gleitman et al pp. 270-271, 275), which we'll get to in class soon enough.

    WEB PAGE

    My web page for PSYC 132:72 is at http://web9.uits.uconn.edu/lundquis/psyc132.html

    From there you can get to the text of the overheads used in class, as well as to Prof. Turvey's old exams (see below). The overheads are by no means an exhaustive account of all the course material, and they don't contain all the detail you will need, but they do serve as an outline of the material and a reminder of some key concepts to know.

    Though putting the following information on the web is a bit paradoxical, here goes anyway:

    For those who don't know how to view a web page: in brief, you get to a computer with an internet connection (in a dorm room, or in one of the computer labs on campus that are available for undergraduate use, or in the library) (NOTE: on the Hartford Campus, try the School of Social Work!) ; you open a web browser program like Netscape or Microsoft Internet Explorer, and in a box near the top of the screen you type in the address printed above and hit Enter (or Return). For more details, you can ask almost anyone who happens to be nearby at the moment. I have been told that most campus computer labs do not allow printing from most web pages, which means you can only look at the posted info, not take it home with you (unless you know how to download it and save it as a text file, which isn't hard -- see below). So, I have also linked my page to UConn's Virtual Classroom, a set of web pages that you can definitely print from. You can reach the Virtual Classroom at: http://virtual.class.uconn.edu/ and if you ever forget that, you can reach it and the library course reserve page and lots of other useful stuff by going to www.uconn.edu, clicking "Information for UConn Students", and then browsing through the choices that appear.

    Downloading web pages: To download a web page from Netscape (or Explorer, which must be pretty much the same) to the computer you're working on (its hard drive or your own disk), go to the File menu (top left of screen), and choose "save as... text". Save it to the desktop if you have a choice, so it's easy to find. The file will appear on the desktop (or wherever you sent it); you can then open a word-processing program like Microsoft Word or whatever, again go to its File menu and choose "open", find your text file and open it up. Edit it so you cut out what you already have or don't want, and only print what you currently need. This is important to do with the overheads, because you'll probably print a ton of stuff multiple times otherwise.

    PROBLEMS?: If you're doing this from a Windows machine (rather than Mac) you may see your saved overhead file as "overhead.html" even if you said "save as... text" above - and when you open it with Word it may be all scrunched together without any line breaks. I think the way to fix that is to simply change the suffix ".html" to ".txt" (just type right over it!) and try to open it again. The reason is that HTML doesn't recognize carriage returns, so separate lines get run together; text files, on the other hand, DO recognize carriage returns, so as long as the .txt suffix is there, Micrososft Word or whatever you're using should have no difficulty setting up the text file correctly.

    OVERHEADS FOR THE CLASS SO FAR

    You can get to this web page from the class web page. It's being updated (from the overheads I prepared for Fall '99) as we progress through the material. Kind of a problem: If you print the whole thing after every class, as it gets bigger by the day, you'll print the beginning eight or ten times by the time the first exam rolls around. Instead, either wait till just before the exam to print it, or download it (see above) and only print what you currently need. Better yet (for you as well as for the trees), don't print it, just copy what you need into your notes!

    These are not "notes" on class! They don't provide a "summary" of the class; all these are are the overheads I display. That means they may depend crucially on things I said in class for explanation (which, with any luck, you recorded in your notebook). Some topics don't appear here at all (for example, the stuff about Parkinson's disease). Be sure you use this as intended - to fill in gaps in your notes and to remind you of what was covered in class. In some cases I've made slight changes to make the overheads a bit more comprehensible out of context, or to adapt them for posting on the web page. But you should definitely consider your notes and textbook readings to be your primary source of information about these topics. Note: any dates listed are approximate; better check them against your notes.

    SOME OLD 132 EXAMS ON ELECTRONIC COURSE RESERVE

    Practice exam-taking using old exams! You can view them on-line through the class web page and on paper at the Homer Babbidge Library Reserve Desk, next to the Circulation Desk near the gated entrance. (Reserve Desk materials can be checked out for 3 hours, I think -- they can't leave the library but you can read them and/or copy them in that time.) Several of Prof. Turvey's 132 exams are available on the internet through the library's Electronic Course Reserve, at http://www.lib.uconn.edu/ECR/ (look under Psychology, then under Turvey PSYC 132). If you forget this page AND the class web page, just remember to go to www.uconn.edu and click "Information for UConn Students". Eventually you'll find your way to the library's course reserve page.

    (NOTE: the Babbidge Library is open to Hartford campus students as well, though obviously the internet provides easier access from the Hartford campus.)

    I don't have a long history of teaching this course so I don't have exams of my own on file. But Prof. Turvey's exams are very useful to look at when studying for MY exams too! Exams for my class will have only multiple choice questions -- NO essay. But you might take a look at his essay exams just to see how well you could answer those questions. (To identify Turvey's optional essay exams clearly you can click on the "psyc132turvey.html" link on that page.) The exams may not be accessible from an off-campus computer address (e.g., @aol.com instead of @uconn.edu), so if there's a problem, try a dorm or library computer.

    On my class web page, Prof. Turvey's PSYC 132 midterm AND final exam from Fall 1995 are combined together and ordered by topic (which explains why the items are out of numerical order and re-use some numbers). I have made them more useful to you by identifying questions according to the topics they address; this will make it easier for you to pick out the questions on his exams that you should be able to answer. I highly recommend that you at least look at those edited exams so you have some idea of what to expect. Some material may not have been covered in the same way. But for the most part, all questions are answerable from a combination of information presented in lecture and in the Gleitman textbook. These exam questions are representative of (1) the material that will be tested, (2) how the questions will be phrased, and (3) the level of detail at which you should be familiar with the course material. On our actual exams (as with Turvey's unedited exams) they will not be clearly segregated by topic.

    EXAM FORMAT

    The midterm exams will each be 50 multiple choice questions drawn from lecture and textbook material. They will be do-able in one (busy) class period -- get there on time! The final will also have 50 multiple choice questions and is scheduled for a two hour block during finals week.

    Don't expect the multiple choice format to mean you'll just be looking through a list of alternatives trying to recognize some familiar information! The emphasis throughout will not be on rote memory for facts and definitions, but rather on conceptual understanding of the material. For example, new material related to the lecture content may be introduced in a question, requiring an application of the knowledge you have gained so far. But not in a scary way.

    MULTIPLE CHOICE QUESTIONS

    Some general points:

    1) There's a reason the instructions on multiple choice exams always say "choose the BEST answer", instead of just "choose the RIGHT answer" or something like that. In multiple choice questions, you always have to evaluate at least two things for each of the choices: 1) the truth of the statement, and 2) its appropriateness as a response to the particular question. Even if more than one choice looks plausible, choose the BEST alternative (one relevant to the question, not just one that may be a true statement on its own)!

    2) Read all the alternatives first -- see if the familiar ones make sense, then the unfamiliar ones, then decide which is BEST. Maybe you don't understand all the choices but you come across one that you absolutely know is right - then it probably doesn't matter if you understand the others. (Unless you get an "all of the above" choice too...)

    3) Be logical with "all / none of the above" - for instance,

    If you're SURE two of the choices are correct, choose "all of the above" even if you're unsure of the third.

    If you're SURE one of the choices is incorrect, DON'T choose "all of the above" no matter what.

    If you're SURE one of the choices is correct, DON'T choose "none of the above" no matter what.

    Well that last one is pretty obvious, but some people just go to pieces over choices like that, for no good reason. Don't think of these as rules to memorize -- you already know them! Basically, remember "all" means ALL and "none" means NONE. If you ever see other more annoying choices like "at least two of the above", just pay close attention and use the same kind of logic. (I don't usually give those more annoying kind of choices on my exams.)

    4) Use information in the test itself if possible - don't let your answers contradict each other (or the other questions). Often you can go back to a question you saw earlier and use information in one of the choices, or even based on the answer you gave, to decide the answer of some other question you're not sure of. In a perfect exam that would never be the case, but most exams aren't perfect.

    5) Don't allow a response bias like "it's about time a 'd' came up" to influence your answer. Amazing how tempting that can be, but it will never help you (unless the test writer did an amazingly careless job!).

    6) A good test, even a multiple choice one, is testing understanding, not just rote learning of simple facts. Choices might be phrased in an accurate but unfamiliar way; you should be able to understand the idea in all its guises, not just familiar sentences about the idea.

    7) Read carefully -- if a question is comparing or contrasting two ideas, the answer has to be true of BOTH ideas, not just one (e.g., if asked something about BOTH classical conditioning and operant conditioning, don't choose an answer that applies perfectly to one but not at all to the other!). If a question asks about the cause of something, the answer has to be something else that happens before it and directly brings about that thing. And so forth. It's not good enough to just pick a choice that's from the same page of your notes as the question topic.

    Now look at this (hopefully easy) example:

    The action potential occurs because
    a. sodium ions enter the neuron
    b. potassium ions enter the neuron
    c. the neuron contains neurotransmitters in its vesicles
    d. all of the above

    The right answer is (a). Answer (b) is incorrect. Answer (c) is a true statement which is irrelevant to the question. A student trying to defend answer (c) would probably say, look, if the neuron didn't contain neurotransmitters, it couldn't release them and cause an action potential in its neighbor across the synapse, so the action potential happens because of that! To which the instructor replies, that's true, but it's clearly not the BEST answer - (a) describes the immediate cause of the change in voltage which gives rise to the action potential. Notice that the question uses that word "because" to ask about the CAUSE; it doesn't just say "which of the statements below has something or other to do with an action potential?"

    If you were hyper-imaginative (and smart, even), you might also choose (b) if you thought, hmm, there's something in my notes about how potassium ions get pushed OUT of the neuron once the charge inside has become highly positive... and if they get pushed out, they must eventually trade places with sodium ions again, which means potassium goes back in at some point ... must be true then! (This would be a case of what some people refer to as "thinking too much".) Well that answer is wrong for the same two reasons: 1) it's not the BEST -- you have to attach lots of other conditions to the statement to even see that it might be true, and 2) it's not a CAUSE - it happens AFTER the action potential.

    (It's not that "cause" is the only important thing to consider; it's just what was asked for in THIS question. Others ask you to focus on similarities, or differences, or definitions, etc., and for those questions you have to be sure you're paying attention to specifically those aspects of the choices.)

    Finally, (d) is wrong since only (a) is right. But that's no big deal: as long as you know for SURE that even one of the choices (say (b)) is incorrect, you'd never put (d) just because you thought maybe (a) and (c) both looked right. "All" means ALL! No reason to get freaked out by those "all / none of the above" answers.

    (If there were another choice that read "the voltage difference decreases from -70 mV to -55 mV", that wouldn't be very different from (a) in its accuracy and relevance; in fact, it would be equally good, and the question would be unfair because not only could you not decide on a single best answer, but you might also be tempted to say "all of the above" since there were two completely correct ones. If I discover a question on an exam is bad like that, I'll go through all the exams and give credit to anyone who chose any possibly correct answer. (If you chose only (b) or (c), you'd still be out of luck!) Luckily for all of us, that almost never happens to me!)

    Some more suggestions for test taking are posted at http://www.socialpsychology.org/testtips.htm , for those who are interested. Personally, I doubt his advice about "first answers are usually correct", popular though that notion is. I also doubt the very last paragraph, which seems to be saying, "Use the Force." But I suppose those bits of advice are better than "eeny, meeny, miney, moe," if you're at a total loss!

    WHAT TO STUDY:

    The lecture material is primary; use the text as a resource to elaborate the lecture topics.

    If something is covered in lecture, you will be responsible for the lecture coverage and ALL of the required text coverage of that topic as listed on the syllabus, unless I specifically tell you to omit certain pages.

    If something is NOT covered in lecture, you will NOT be responsible for the text coverage of that topic, unless I specifically tell you to study certain pages.

    Page numbers for all required reading are listed on the syllabus. Pages labeled OPTIONAL will not be on the exams; these are just portions of the text that I would have lectured on given more time, so I've referred you to them in case you want to pursue additional reading on any of those topics. (Hey, it could happen.)

    The Gleitman text isn't just a really long story that you read through repeatedly. You should read all the assigned material, probably at least twice. But then also use the index to find answers to particular questions that come up in your notes, your reading, or the old exams. Use the summary points at the end of the chapters, and the contents at the beginning of chapters, to help you identify what's there and how it's organized.